Volume 73, No.1: 2021 Siriraj Medical Journal
https://he02.tci-thaijo.org/index.php/sirirajmedj/index
24
One possible explanation of our ndings was visual
adaptation through perceptual learning.
33
Dierent coping
mechanism might be used to overcome the hindrances
caused by visual eld defect strategy. When the central
vision was compromised, eccentric xation might be
used in the visual rehabilitation of the visual eld defect.
Scanning involving parafoveal and peripheral visual
eld is crucial to navigating reading. Patients with visual
eld defects might have adapted to the condition with
adjusted eye and head movements or compensatory
gaze strategies to improve reading performance.
33,38,39
Fixations and saccades denote how readers acquire
information. Visual attention can be in an engaged or
disengaged state.
40
To move from one point to another,
visual attention should be in the disengaged state. During
engaged visual attention, saccades were inhibited to
provide steady central xation. e disengaged mechanism
seemed to be intact (insignicant saccadic nding in
our study) during visual search in reading despite visual
eld defects. A dierent coping inclination between eye
movement adjustment and reading adaptation might
occur in response to visual eld defects. Future research
with additional measurements on the time length of
each eye movement is essential. Time length for saccade
can estimate how fast the eye moves between xations.
Time length for regression predicts the eort required
to reread a line of text.
In conclusion, reading speed and xation were
aected by dierent types of glaucomatous visual eld
defects patterns. e association of defect areas with
faster reading speed but not signicant in eye movements
might suggest a possible dierent coping strategy between
eye movement adjustment and reading adaptation in
response to visual eld defects.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
is study was funded by the Research Entity Initiative
[600-IRMI/REI 5/3 (016/2018)]. anks to Prof Liza-
Sharmini Ahmad Tajudin for constructive comments.
Special thanks to Noor Halilah Buari & Ethan Hoe Tzong
Shuen for technical supports.
Conict of interest: All authors have no conict of
interest.
REFERENCES
1. Pammer K, Hansen PC, Kringelbach ML, Holliday IE, Barnes
G, Hillebrand A, et al. Visual word recognition: the rst half
second. Neuroimage 2004;22:1819-25.
2. Singleton C, Henderson LM. Visual factors in reading. London
Rev Education 2006;4:89-98.
3. Maharajah KR, Tet CM, Yaacob A, Tajudin LSA, Foster PJ.
Modied Bahasa Malaysia version of VF-14 questionnaire:
assessing the impact of glaucoma in rural area of Malaysia.
Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2008;36:222-31.
4. Ramulu P. Glaucoma and disability: which tasks are aected,
and at what stage of disease? Curr Opin Ophthalmol 2009;20:
92-8.
5. Lester M, Zingirin M. Quality of life in patients with early,
moderate and advanced glaucoma. Eye 2002;16:44-9.
6. Freeman EE, Muñoz B, West SK, Jampel HD, Friedman DS.
Glaucoma and quality of life: the Salisbury Eye Evaluation.
Ophthalmol 2008;115:233-8.
7. Murata H, Hirasawa H, Aoyama Y, Sugisaki K, Araie M,
Mayama C, et al. Identifying areas of the visual eld important
for quality of life in patients with glaucoma. PLOS ONE
2013;8:e58695.
8. Ishii M, Seki M, Harigai R, Abe H, Fukuchi T. Reading performance
in patients with glaucoma evaluated using the MNREAD
charts. Jpn J Ophthalmol 2013;57:471-4.
9. Ramulu PY, West SK, Munoz B, Jampel HD, Friedman DS.
Glaucoma and reading speed. Arch Ophthalmol 2009;127:82-7.
10. Roberts K, Haymes S, Leblanc R, Nicolela M, Chauhan B, Artes
P. Contrast sensitivity, visual acuity, reading speed and macular
visual eld damage in glaucoma. Investig Ophthalmol & Vis
Sci 2005;46:4665.
11. Tabrett DR, Latham K. Important areas of the central binocular
visual eld for daily functioning in the visually impaired.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2012;32:156-63.
12. Altangerel U, Spaeth GL, Steinmann WC. Assessment of Function
Related to Vision (AFREV). Ophthalmic Epidemiol 2006;13:
67-80.
13. Burton R, Crabb DP, Smith ND, Glen FC, Garway-Heath DF.
Glaucoma and reading: exploring the eects of contrast lowering
of text. Optom Vis Sci 2012;89:1282-7.
14. Burton R, Saunders LJ, Crabb DP. Areas of the visual eld
important during reading in patients with glaucoma. Jpn J
Ophthalmol 2015;59:94-102.
15. Whittaker SG, Lovie-Kitchin J. Visual requirements for reading.
Optom Vis Sci 1993;70:54-65.
16. Burton R, Smith ND, Crabb DP. Eye movements and reading
in glaucoma: observations on patients with advanced visual
eld loss. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2014;252:1621-
30.
17. Krieber M, Bartl-Pokorny K, Pokorny F, Einspieler C, Langmann
A, Korner C, et al. e Relation between reading skills and eye
movement patterns in adolescent readers: evidence from a
regular orthography. PLoS One 2016;11:e145934.
18. Rayner K. Eye movements in reading and information processing:
20 years of research. Psychol Bull 1998;124:372-422.
19. Jampel HD, Friedman DS, Quigley H, Miller R. Correlation
of the binocular visual eld with patient assessment of vision.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2002;43:1059-67.
20. Cummings R, Rubin G. Reading speed and saccadic eye movements
with an articial paracentral scotoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis
Sci 1993;34:1318.
21. Buari NH, Chen AH. Buari-Chen Malay Reading Chart (BCMRC):
Contextual Sentence and Random Words 2-in-1 Design in
Malay Language. Pertanika J Sci & Technol 2017;25:135-50.
Chen et al.