Mindsets among Sixth Grade Students and their Correlation with Academic Achievement and School-related Happiness

Main Article Content

Khemika Khemakanok Sudnawa
Chakriya Theeranate
Apirom Yailaibang

Abstract

Background: Mindset Theory has illustrated that people hold either “fixed” (intelligence is static) or “growth” (intelligence can be developed) mindsets and that these views may affect people’s emotions, behaviors, and finally outcomes. Many studies have shown that growth mindset is a positive influence on academic success and happiness. However, little is known about the mindsets among primary school students, and how mindset could affect their academic achievement and happiness.


Objective:      The study aimed to determine children’s mindsets and any associations and correlations between mindset and academic achievement, measured by the Ordinary National Educational Test (O-NET), and school-related happiness using a school-related happiness questionnaire among 6th grade students.


Methods:       We conducted a cross-sectional analytic study of 6th grade students from December 1st, 2017 - April 30th, 2018. After obtaining informed consent, we collected demographic data, identified mindset, and determined school-related happiness using questionnaires. At the end of the trimester, we collected O-NET scores in 4 subjects (Math, Science, Thai, and English). The results were analyzed by descriptive statistical methods, One-way MANOVA with post hoc analysis, and simple regression analysis.


Results:         The total number of participants was 431, which were classified as growth mindset (56%), undecided (38%), and fixed mindset (6%). Growth mindset was significantly associated with higher O-NET scores in all 4 subjects and school-related happiness scores (P<0.05). However, the mindset had low correlation with school-related happiness scores (b 0.529, adjust R2 0.278, p < 0.001), and very low correlation with the total O-NET scores (b 0.173, adjust R2 0.028, p < 0.001). In addition, the mindset had very low correlation with Thai, Math, and Science but wasn’t correlated with English. When controlling other variables, the mindset could predict the total O-NET scores by 3%, and school-related happiness scores by 28%.


Conclusion: Among sixth grade students, growth mindset was the predominant mindset. Growth mindset was associated with higher O-NET scores and school-related happiness scores. However, mindset had low correlation with school-related happiness scores and very low correlation with the total O-NET scores.

Article Details

Section
นิพนธ์ต้นฉบับ (Original Article)

References

1. Wegner LM. school achievement and underachievement In: Carey WB, Crocker AC, Coleman WL, Elias ER, Feldman HM, editors. Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics. 4th ed. Philadelphia: Suanders Elsevier;2009. p. 497-505.
2. Gutman LM, Schoon I. The impact of non-cognitive skills on outcomes for young people. London: Education Endowment Foundation; 2013.
3. Dweck CS, Master A. Self-relations, self-esteem, self-care, and self-control. In: Carey WB, Crocker AC, Coleman WL, Elias ER, Feldman HM, editors. Developmental-behavioral pediatrics. 4th ed. philadelphia: Saunders Elsevier; 2009. p. 427-35.
4. Dweck CS. Self- theories: their role in motivation, personality, and development. New york: Psychology press; 2000.
5. Dweck CS. Mindset the new psychology of success. New york: Ballantine book; 2008.
6. Dweck CS, Leggett EL. A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychol Rev. 1988;95(2):256-73.
7. Blackwell LS, Trzesniewski KH, Dweck CS. Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across and adolescent transition: a longitudinal study and an intervention. child Dev. 2007;78(1):246-63.
8. Romero C, Master A, Paunesku D, Dweck CS, Gross JJ. Academic and emotional functioning in middle school: the role of implicit theories. Emotion. 2014;14(2):227-34.
9. Stipek D, Gralinski-Bakker JH. Children's beliefs about intelligence and school performance. J Educ Psychol. 1996;88(3):397-407.
10. Claro S, Paunesku D, Dweck CS. Growth mindset tempers the effects of poverty on academic achievement. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(31):8664-8.
11. Costa A, Faria L. Implicit Theories of Intelligence and Academic Achievement: A Meta-Analytic Review. Front Psychol. 2018;9:829.
12. Aronson J, Fried CB, Good C. Reducing the Effects of Stereotype Threat on African American College Students by Shaping Theories of Intelligence. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2001;38(2):113-25.
13. Good C, Aronson J, Inzlicht M. Improving adolescents' standardized test performance: An intervention to reduce the effects of stereotype threat. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2003;24(6):645-62.
14. Paunesku D, Walton GM, Romero C, Smith EN, Yeager DS, Dweck CS. Mind-set interventions are a scalable treatment for academic underachievement. Psychol Sci. 2015;26(6):784-93.
15. Yeager DS, Romero C, Paunesku D, Hulleman CS, Schneider B, Hinojosa C, et al. Using Design Thinking to Improve Psychological Interventions: The Case of the Growth Mindset During the Transition to High School. J Educ Psychol. 2016;108(3):374-91.
16. Jegathesan M, Vitberg YM, Pusic MV. A survey of mindset theories of intelligence and medical error self-reporting among pediatric housestaff and facculty. BMC med educ. 2016;16(58):1-6.
17. Ivens J. The Development of a Happiness Measure for Schoolchildren. Educ Psychol Pract. 2007;23(3):221-39.
18. Second language learning In: Lightbown PM, Spada N, editors. How languages are learned. Oxford: Oxford university press; 2013. p. 35-122.
19. King RB. Fixed mindset leads to negative affect: The relations between implicit theories of intelligence and subjective well-being. Z Psychol. 2017;225(2):137-45.