Long-Term Vipassana Meditation Enhances Executive Function in Adult Meditators

Authors

  • Thawatchai Lukseng Research Center for Neuroscience, Institute of Molecular Biosciences, Mahidol University, Nakornpathom 73170 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-5226-5225
  • Vorasith Siripornpanich Research Center for Neuroscience, Institute of Molecular Biosciences, Mahidol University, Nakornpathom 73170 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6614-4520
  • Nuanchan Chutabhakdikul Research Center for Neuroscience, Institute of Molecular Biosciences, Mahidol University, Nakornpathom 73170 http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7338-4824

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.33192/Smj.2020.47

Keywords:

Vipassana meditation, executive function, Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Digit Span, Tower of Hanoi

Abstract

Objective: Vipassana meditation (VM) is a traditional Buddhist meditation practice that focuses on monitoring of clear awareness of oneself exactly what is happening as it happens, without judging. Executive functions (EF) are the high-level cognitive processes that facilitate goal-directed behaviors. It is well known that VM has significant effects on various affective states of the mind, such as relaxation, reduce stress and anxiety; however, less is known about the effect of VM on the executive function. This study aims to examine the effects of VM practice on the performance of the executive function in the adult meditators.
Methods:
Forty adult participants, age range between 25-50 year-olds, were recruited to this study. They were divided into three groups; the control group (N=20, mean age = 40.5 ± 5.8 years), Short-term VM group (N=6, mean age = 38.0±9.1 years), and Long-term VM group (N=14, mean age = 37.7±7.3 years). All participants were examined by 1) State-trait anxiety inventory (STAI); 2) Philadelphia mindfulness scale (PHLMS); 3) Digit span task of WAIS-IV, 4) Tower of Hanoi (ToH), and 5) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST-CV4). The mean scores of all task performance were statistically analyzed and compared between groups. Alpha values of .05 were considered significant throughout.
Results:
Both short-term and long-term VM has common benefits to decreased anxiety and increased mindfulness score as compared with the non-meditator group. Although short-term VM shows some benefits to the performance of several EF tasks, the discrepancy was not significant when compared with the control group. In contrast, long-term VM had a significant benefit to the performance of working memory, planning, and shift/cognitive flexibility, when compared with the non-meditator group. Our results indicated that long-term VM practice not only reduces anxiety and improves mindfulness, but the benefit also extends to improve the performance of the executive function in adult practitioners.
Conclusion:
In conclusion, our results suggest that continued practice of VM is highly effective for enhancing EF in healthy individuals. Long-term VM practice not only reduce stress and improve mindfulness but also enhance the performance of EF tasks of the practitioners.

References

1. Cahn BR, Polich J. Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol Bull 2006;132(2):180-211.
2. Varanavin S. The Result of Meditation Practice on Emotional Quotient : A Case Study of The Dhamma Practitioner According To His Majesty The King’s Exemplary Project At Wat Somanas Vihara. Bangkok: Mahachulalongkornrajavidyalaya University; 2007.
3. Chiesa A, Serretti A. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for stress management in healthy people: a review and meta-analysis. J Altern Complement Med 2009;15(5):593-600.
4. Marchand WR. Mindfulness-based stress reduction, mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, and Zen meditation for depression, anxiety, pain, and psychological distress. J Psychiatr Pract 2012;18(4):233-52.
5. Chen KW, Berger CC, Manheimer E, Forde D, Magidson J, Dachman L, et al. Meditative therapies for reducing anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Depress Anxiety 2012;29(7):545-62.
6. Ganpat TS, Nagendra HR, Muralidhar K. Effects of yoga on brain wave coherence in executives. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 2011;55(4):304-8.
7. van der Riet P. Vipassana meditation: one woman's narrative. Collegian 2011;18(1):36-42.
8. Chan D, Woollacott M. Effects of level of meditation experience on attentional focus: is the efficiency of executive or orientation networks improved? J Altern Complement Med 2007;13(6):651-7.
9. Rubia K. The neurobiology of Meditation and its clinical effectiveness in psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychol 2009;82(1):1-11.
10. Zgierska A, Rabago D, Chawla N, Kushner K, Koehler R, Marlatt A. Mindfulness meditation for substance use disorders: a systematic review. Subst Abus 2009;30(4):266-94.
11. Grant JA, Duerden EG, Courtemanche J, Cherkasova M, Duncan GH, Rainville P. Cortical thickness, mental absorption and meditative practice: possible implications for disorders of attention. Biol Psychol 2013;92(2):275-81.
12. Lutz A, Slagter HA, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ. Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn Sci 2008;12(4):163-9.
13. Travis F, Shear J. Focused attention, open monitoring and automatic self-transcending: Categories to organize meditations from Vedic, Buddhist and Chinese traditions. Conscious Cogn 2010;19(4):1110-8.
14. Mars TS, Abbey H. Mindfulness meditation practise as a healthcare intervention: A systematic review. International Journal of Osteopathic Medicine 2010;13(2):56-66.
15. Chiesa A. Vipassana meditation: systematic review of current evidence. J Altern Complement Med 2010;16(1):37-46.
16. Lippelt DP, Hommel B, Colzato LS. Focused attention, open monitoring and loving kindness meditation: effects on attention, conflict monitoring, and creativity - A review. Front Psychol 2014;5:1083.
17. Hasenkamp W, Barsalou LW. Effects of meditation experience on functional connectivity of distributed brain networks. Front Hum Neurosci 2012;6:38.
18. Farb NA, Segal ZV, Anderson AK. Mindfulness meditation training alters cortical representations of interoceptive attention. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013;8(1):15-26.
19. Wimmer L, Bellingrath S, von Stockhausen L. Cognitive Effects of Mindfulness Training: Results of a Pilot Study Based on a Theory Driven Approach. Front Psychol 2016;7:1037.
20. Young KS, van der Velden AM, Craske MG, Pallesen KJ, Fjorback L, Roepstorff A, et al. The impact of mindfulness-based interventions on brain activity: A systematic review of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2018;84:424-33.
21. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol 2013;64:135-68.
22. Gilbert SJ, Burgess PW. Executive function. Curr Biol 2008;18(3):R110-4.
23. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 2000;41(1):49-100.
24. Zelazo PD, Craik FI, Booth L. Executive function across the life span. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2004;115(2-3):167-83.
25. Lengacher CA, Johnson-Mallard V, Post-White J, Moscoso MS, Jacobsen PB, Klein TW, et al. Randomized controlled trial of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) for survivors of breast cancer. Psychooncology 2009;18(12):1261-72.
26. Balaji PA, Varne SR, Ali SS. Physiological effects of yogic practices and transcendental meditation in health and disease. N Am J Med Sci 2012;4(10):442-8.
27. Jain S, Shapiro SL, Swanick S, Roesch SC, Mills PJ, Bell I, et al. A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation versus relaxation training: effects on distress, positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction. Ann Behav Med 2007;33(1):11-21.
28. Kral TRA, Schuyler BS, Mumford JA, Rosenkranz MA, Lutz A, Davidson RJ. Impact of short- and long-term mindfulness meditation training on amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli. Neuroimage 2018;181:301-13.
29. Spielberger CD. State-trait anxiety inventory for adults. Sampler set: Palo Alto, CA: Mind Garden, [1983] ©1983; 1983.
30. Silpakit C, Silpakit O, Wisajun P. The validity of Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale Thai version. Journal of Mental Health of Thailand 2011;19(3):140-7.
31. Zeng X, Li M, Zhang B, Liu X. Revision of the Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale for Measuring Awareness and Equanimity in Goenka's Vipassana Meditation with Chinese Buddhists. J Relig Health 2014.
32. Hartman DE. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IV (WAIS IV): return of the gold standard. Appl Neuropsychol 2009;16(1):85-7.
33. Robert KH. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST): Revised and expanded. Odessa, Fla. (P.O. Box 998, Odessa 33556) : Psychological Assessment Resources, [1993] ©1993; 1993.
34. Slagter HA, Lutz A, Greischar LL, Francis AD, Nieuwenhuis S, Davis JM, et al. Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biol 2007;5(6):e138.
35. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci 2005;9(5):242-9.
36. Ling-Yin Liang, Patricia A. Shewokis, Getchell N. Brain Activation in the Prefrontal Cortex during Motor and Cognitive Tasks in Adults. Behavioral and Brain Science 2016;6:463-74.
37. Pochon JB, Levy R, Poline JB, Crozier S, Lehericy S, Pillon B, et al. The role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the preparation of forthcoming actions: an fMRI study. Cereb Cortex 2001;11(3):260-6.
38. Tomasino B, Fabbro F. Increases in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and decreases the rostral prefrontal cortex activation after-8 weeks of focused attention based mindfulness meditation. Brain Cogn 2016;102:46-54.

Downloads

Published

19-05-2020

How to Cite

Lukseng, T. ., Siripornpanich, V. ., & Chutabhakdikul, N. . (2020). Long-Term Vipassana Meditation Enhances Executive Function in Adult Meditators. Siriraj Medical Journal, 72(4), 343–351. https://doi.org/10.33192/Smj.2020.47

Issue

Section

Original Article